“The Science of Reading”
The Importance of Translating Research to Practice
By Kathryn Walters & Zahaira J. Cruz Aponte
Over a century ago, scientists discovered that our eyes are constantly moving from point to point, and perceived information is only sent to the brain when the eyes are still. This had big implications for the study of reading; the community was flipped on its head! Whereas previously philosophical approaches predominated our understanding of reading, now reading scholars were asking more scientific questions, and working to answer them using machines that would lead to advances like the modern-day eye tracker. In short, the study of reading became a science.
Since reading is in many ways essential for navigating the world, it is important that we understand how it works. This is particularly true when it comes to teaching young kids how to read: Unlike language, reading doesn’t come naturally through exposure, so understanding how the reading process works is essential for helping young learners acquire this important skill. However, despite over a hundred years of science-based reading research, the educational community has been slow to adapt to research-based practices. This has been to the detriment of many young students, leading to the current “Science of Reading” movement, which aims to bring insights from reading research into practice [1]. In this article we touch on some aspects of this movement and the research that motivates it and discuss how it provides a case study for why it is important to integrate research into our educational practices.
Our understanding of how reading works is the result of research from multiple disciplines, including psychology, education, communication sciences, neuroscience, and linguistics [1]. One of the important insights we have learned from reading science is that, unlike language, reading is “unnatural” and “artificial” [2]. What this means is that reading is something that must be learned through explicit instruction, as a skill. Science has taught us that children cannot learn to read simply through exposure to books, and they must be given tools to make sense out of the print in front of them. Research has shown quite clearly that understanding how sounds connect to letters (e.g., ‘a’ is for apple, ‘b’ is for bananas) is essential for reading [1]. In some languages, like Spanish, this relationship is fairly straightforward, while in others, like English it is less so. (See our fun piece in this newsletter for more on this!)
Despite the knowledge that connecting letters and sounds is essential for reading, for decades, predominant ways of teaching reading have left explicit instruction on this fundamental aspect of reading out of the curriculum. Instead, reading curricula used in most schools have focused on encouraging children to use context and other elements of the text to figure out what a word says [3]. However, it has now become clear that ignoring the science in this regard has been to the detriment of many emergent readers. In recent years, this has led to a ‘revolt’, led in large part by families and individuals who feel they were sold short by a school system that ignored results from science and therefore failed them in their reading instruction [4]. The science of reading movement emerged from this, as a demand to pay attention to the science and integrate research-based practices into how we teach reading.
There are numerous connections between the science of reading and language science, which is the focus of our research here at the Center for Language Science. One of these connections has been made between children’s understanding of the fundamental abilities associated with reading and the process of learning to read, such as knowledge of the alphabet. When children have a better awareness of the sounds around them, their understanding of the alphabet and how these sounds connect to letters tends to be enriched. This knowledge in turn helps in making the process of learning to read quicker and more efficient. And in turn, learning how to read in a more efficient way helps in improving overall language skills. This process can ultimately lead to gains that extend well beyond reading and language abilities, which is evident when one considers the many doors that both reading and language may open for individuals [5,6].
Looking beyond reading and language, there is much to be learned from the science of reading movement itself. The current movement shows us that, when we ignore lessons from science, this may be to the detriment of society. On the other hand, the path to integrating science into practice can be messy, and the correct way forward is not always clear. By conducting and, importantly, sharing reading and language research with the public in meaningful ways, we can contribute to more informed decisions and practices, and ultimately, to a better shared understanding of the world we live in. Such sharing is essential if we wish for our research to contribute toward improving the lives of and increasing the opportunities available to both current and future generations.
References
[1] Johns, A. 2023. The Science of Reading: Information, Media, and Mind in Modern America. The University of Chicago Press.
[2] Huey, Edmund Burke. 1908. The Psychology and Pedagogy of Reading. The Macmillan company [c1908] 1916. https://babel.hathitrust.org/cgi/pt?id=umn.31951d007276859&seq=1
[3] Mervosh, Sarah. 2023. Kids Can’t Read,’ and the Education Establishment Faces a Revolt. The New York Times, April 16, 2023. https://www.nytimes.com/2023/04/16/us/science-of-reading-literacy-parents.html
[4] Hanford, E (presenter). (2022, October 20). The Problem. [Episode of a podcast]. In Sold a Story. APM Reports. https://podcasts.apple.com/us/podcast/sold-a-story/id1649580473
[5] Lafrance, Adele. and Gottardo, Alexandra. 2005. “A longitudinal study of phonological processing skills and reading in bilingual children.” Applied Psycholinguistics, 26(4), 559–578. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050307
[6] Dickinson, David K., Griffith, Julie A., Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick, & Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy. 2012. “How reading books fosters language development around the world.” Child Development Research Article 602807. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/602807