Monthly Archives: March 2014

LIFE WITHOUT NEW MEMORIES

Two years ago, I got a phone call from a college friend which brought the devastation of working memory loss into painful reality.  At that time, I was told that my former college roommate, now only thirty years old, had been hospitalized after being found in the final life threatening stages of a battle with alcoholism.  Once she regained consciousness and was physically stabilized, it was clear that permanent brain damage remained.  This was my introduction to the condition known as Korsakoff’s psychosis/syndrome, a result of chronic and severe alcoholism.  I was unaware of this sad syndrome until my old friend B was diagnosed with it.

Karsakoff’s syndrome is the second stage of a brain disorder known as Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome, a result of vitamin B1 (thiamine) insufficiency.  The thiamine deficiency in alcoholics is a result of their characteristic poor diet which is low in vitamins, as well alcohol’s interference with thiamine absorption in the intestines.  Karsakoff’s syndrome is characterized by a person’s inability to form new memories, otherwise known as anterograde amnesia.  (Goldstein, 149)  Anterograde amnesia is often coupled with retrograde amnesia, which is memory loss affecting past events.  In Beth’s case, she experienced symptoms of both retrograde and anterograde amnesia (global amnesia).  She had some recall of past events and semantic memories but no retention at all of new memories.  Karsakoff’s psychosis results from bilateral lesions of the medial portion of the temporal lobe, specifically the hippocampus, and widespread loss of nerve cells. (Myers)

Korsakoff’s syndrome causes deficits in declarative memory in most patients, but keeps implicit spatial, verbal, and procedural memory functioning intact. (alzheimers.org)  This was clear in Beth’s case.  Inside her hospital room, she saw and could recognize her friends and family.  She held on a conversation with all the same nuances she had twelve years before, asking all the “right” questions.  However, her deficits in declarative memory made functional conversation impossible.  Our conversation two weeks after of hospitalization included normal dialogue from her such as,

“How are your parents?”

“What is new?”

“You have four kids, I don’t believe it!”

However, conversational norms ended there.  Every five minutes or so, the same good natured questions would repeat…

“How are your parents?”

“What is new?”

And then she would react as though these responses she had heard five minutes before were new and B would reply,

“You have four kids, I don’t believe it!”

Additionally it was difficult to have a twice sided conversation because she could answer no questions about present circumstances and very few about recent long term circumstances.

“Was she still working at her last job?”  She couldn’t remember.

“Who visited you this morning?”  She wasn’t quite sure.

She would start to tell a story and stop abruptly, searching for information that was clearly lost to her.  You could see that she was frustrated when she could not retrieve a memory.  This made sense, because of all of our friends in college B was the brightest and most knowledgeable, and had been able to hold an interesting conversation with anyone.

Korsakoff’s syndrome is not curable. (alzheimers.org) The brain damage done is irreversible and perhaps the conversation above sheds some light on how difficult it is for people with this condition to function on their own.  B cannot remember if she has had food to eat, what day it is, turned off the stove, or has taken her medication.  Many of the small things that we need to remember to make life as we know it possible involve being able to create and retain new memories, without them, we are rendered helpless.  Like most others with this condition, B will be institutionalized for the rest of her life because her inability to form new memories makes life on her own impossible.

 

WORKS CITED

Goldstein, B. E. (2011). Cognitive Psychology Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience, Third Edition.  Belmont, CA:Wadsworth.

Myers, Catherine E.  (2006) Memory Loss and the Brain. (Web) Retrieved 14 Mar 2014 from             http://www.memorylossonline.com. 

What is Karsoff’s Syndrome. (2012) Alzheimer’s Society. (Web) Retrieved 14 Mar 2014 from             http://www.alzheimers.org.uk.

 

Photographs: Forgetting to Remember?

We so often take for granted what is right in front of us.  One common way this happens in society today is through the use of cameras that we think will help us to remember important events or objects.  Yet what if that photo actually impaired your ability to remember the object or scene you are photographing?  According the Dr. Linda Henkel, it just might.  To understand how this is possible I have taken a look at an article about Dr. Linda Henkel’s research with our lessons on attention and memory in mind, as well as describing some happenings in my life that illustrate this phenomenon.

In the article, we learned that Dr. Linda Henkel speculates that people are not able to fully attend to what is in front of them because they are instead attending to the action of taking a photo when this occurs.  It is almost as if we expect the technology to do the remembering for us.  Dr. Henkel tested this theory, which she calls the “photo-taking impairment effect,” by having a group of students take photos of different exhibits in a museum and then seeing how much they could actually remember about the objects the following day.  The results seemed to support her theory very strongly and the findings were able to be replicated later on.

It is hard for us to say with certainty which theory of attention is correct, as it seems to change along with the circumstance.  In my opinion, Dr. Linda Henkel’s work seems to coincide with Broadbent’s model the most.  This would mean that information that is to be paid attention to is filtered out very early in the process.  It seems to me that when you “know” you will remember something because you have a camera, your brain puts a stricter filter on what you actually pay attention to because you feel as if you can look at the photo later and easily recall the experience. It also occurs to me that this attentional filter would be located right in between the sensory memory and the short-term memory if this is the case.  We do not feel the need to recall the information or process it any further, so we simply let it slip of out our minds rather than store it in the STM and rehearse it so it can move on the long-term memory.

I can relate this to an experience in my own life, which really makes me feel as if Dr. Henkel is on to something.  Last June my little sister got married.  I was her maid of honor.  While we were at her bridal shower, I focused on taking a lot of photographs so she could remember her shower.  Now looking back on the shower, I find I have a hard time remembering many details such as what we ate, what the cake look liked, and what gifts I watched her open.  It seems that I took up so much of my attentional capacity with the photo-taking that I was unable to fully attend to anything else that day.

In conclusion, I feel that Dr. Henkel is really on the right track with her theory of “photo-taking impairment effect.”  I hate to think that all the years I have been taking photos of things I wanted to remember were actually spent helping myself to forget.  However, with that in mind I plan to be more conscious of the world around me each day and never let anything pass me by simply because I feel the need to photograph it.  Instead I think I will first fully attend to the person/object/situation without a camera and once I have taken it all in, I will then take a photograph if I still feel it necessary.

Works Cited:

Goldstein, E. Bruce. Cognitive Psychology. Australia: Wadsworth Cengage Learning, 2011. Print.

Knapton, Sarah. “Taking Photographs Ruins the Memory, Research Finds.” The Telegraph. Telegraph Media Group, 10 Dec. 2013. Web. 15 Mar. 2014. <http://www.telegraph.co.uk/science/science-news/10507146/Taking-photographs-ruins-the-memory-research-finds.html>.

Memories of my Dad

What is your earliest memory?  Was it traumatic?  Does it involve an influential figure or monumental event in your life?  According to our textbook the definition of memory is, “the processes involved in retaining, retrieving, and using information about stimuli, images, events, ideas, and skills, after the original information is no longer present.”

My Dad had passed away three months shy of my third birthday. I have three distinctive memories surrounding his death.  In the first one, he was balancing my bottle on his stomach and every time he took a breath my bottle would fall to the side and I remember laughing over and over like it was the funniest thing I had ever seen.  The second is my Mom standing over his casket and placing a tender kiss to his cheek leaving an imprint of her red lipstick.  The third is of me and my sister in her bedroom and as we sat upon her bed I had ask her where Daddy was and through fallen tears she had explained to me that he went to heaven.  I had never talked about my memories or asked if anybody else remembers them actually happening.  The memories are mine and I don’t want to share them or realize that they never happened although I often wonder if they are actually true or if somewhere between then and now I had heard stories from someone or just made them up to cope with not knowing personally who my Dad was.  I can remember specifics such as the exact color of my Mom’s lipstick and the tears running down my sister’s face.  I remember him being shirtless as my bottle bounced off of the black couch in our den.  I don’t remember what triggered the memories.  They have just always simply been there.

The memories are episodic, which means that they are from personal experience and allows me to travel back in time to when the events took place (Goldstein 2011).  I think about these memories quite often as to not forget.  I don’t have flashbacks or anxiety when thinking about these events.  I cherish them, although they were not a pleasant time.  They give me a sense of peace knowing that even though my Dad isn’t here I have something small and significant to hold on to.

 

References

Goldstein, B. (2011). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 3rd Edition. Wadsworth, Inc.

Concussions in High Risk Sports (Equestrian)

Concussions are a controversial topic in the equestrian community. Participation in high risk sports increases the chance of injury. A fall from two feet can cause permanent brain damage: horses elevate riders eight feet or more above the ground.  A human skull can be shattered by an impact of 4-6 mph: horses are capable of galloping at 40 mph. Head injuries account for approximately 60% of equestrian related deaths, Despite these overwhelming statistics, many riders still choose to refrain from wearing a helmet.

Last week Grand Prix dressage rider Silva Martin was hospitalized following a freak accident and sustained a serious head injury. Many argue that only top-level competitors who are jumping need to wear a helmet. However, Silva was only teaching a lesson off of the horse when it tripped and fell on her. Luckily she was wearing a helmet, but was still knocked unconscious and suffered a seizure.

Immediately after the accident, Silva had been drifting in and out of consciousness and was having trouble moving her left arm and leg. The following day she showed some signs of improvement but remained in the same condition. However, the bleeding and swelling on her brain was also affecting her eyesight. Based on these symptoms, it appears that the back of Silva’s brain suffered the most damage. The cerebellum controls movement is located below the cerebral cortex and above the brain stem. Similarly, the occipital lobe which is responsible for vision is also located at the back of the brain.

According to the text, amnesia caused by trauma or brain damage can affect both the ability to form new memories  and the ability to remember events that occurred prior to the injury. In an interview, her husband said that for some reason she thinks that it’s 1988, but she knows who he is! Silva knows her name and is able to squeeze the doctors hand but other things aren’t quite correct. Typically retrograde amnesia from concussions is less severe for remote memories, those that occurred long ago. This could explain Silva’s “selective” memory.

At this point it is too soon to know if she will suffer any longterm effects from the accident. The doctors are optimistic that she will make a recovery but the rate of improvement is very slow. As time passes, consolidation stabilizes memory for the event so it is less likely to be affected by trauma. This process transforms new memories (which are fragile) to a more permanent state where they are resistant to disruption. Consolidation involves a reorganization in the nervous system which occurs at two levels: synaptic and systems. The first occurs at synapses and happens rapidly over a period of minutes. Systems consolidation involves the gradual reorganization of circuits within brain regions and takes place on a longer time scale (weeks, months, or years).

Daddy’s Home!

My husband works the evening shift and, for a while, was getting home around the same time every night.  We have a 5-year-old English Lab, Hershey, who has truly become a “Daddy’s boy” and is always anxiously awaiting the arrival of his human at the end of the night.  He seems to know exactly what time that is, too!  It began some time ago, first with seeing Daddy come through the door every night. Then, whenever I would hear the garage door opening and closing, I would say “Daddy’s home!” and before the door to the house would open, Hershey would be right there, wagging his tail in anticipation.  Eventually, he picked up on the sound of the garage door opening and would run to the door before I even said anything.  Now, as the “magic hour” approaches, he runs to the door and waits — even before Daddy has pulled up in his truck — watching the door intently.  I’ve witnessed this development blossom over time and have been amazed. I couldn’t help but wonder if animals really do have some sort of an “internal clock”, and if their cognitive processes really are similar to that of humans (and if so, to which degree).

Obviously, there had to be some element of priming involved in Hershey’s associating the opening of the door with my husband’s arrival, as well as the later association with the sound of the garage door. Over time, he had learned that these sounds were the precursor to Daddy walking through the door. But what’s behind his ability to know that it’s time to head to the door and wait, without the prior cues of the sound of the garage door or the entry door opening? And how is it that my husband can walk outside to take out the garbage and return a few short minutes later, walk in through that same door, yet Hershey doesn’t get excited like he would had Daddy been gone for 9 hours?

waiting

In searching for answers I found there isn’t very much information out there, however it is said that animals do not appear to have an episodic memory like we humans do.  Using our episodic memory, we are able to engage in mental time travel and develop a sense of time perception by relating events to things that have occurred in the past – mentally estimating that an hour has passed versus a minute, and the ability to differentiate between morning and night. We can plan ahead and anticipate future events based on our recall of past events.  In addition, episodic memory allows us to recall past events with great detail – remembering the who, what, where, when and why of these events.  There has been some debate over whether or not animals live only in the present, hence lacking a sense of future time or events. 

One particular study involving both pigeons and rats spawned this conclusion. When given the choice between an immediate reward or a future reward, the animals opted for the immediate reward — despite the immediate reward being a smaller one and the future reward being a larger one (McGrath, n.d.). While we humans have the ability to learn that a larger reward will be available if we wait it out, animals cannot concieve this.

So, if animals cannot plan the future, how does Hershey know it’s time for Daddy to come home? Research has shown that although animals may lack episodic memory and therefore cannot recall explicit details of a past event, they can still estimate how much time has passed since a particular event has occurred (“Do dogs understand the concept of time?”, n.d.). They can tell that it has been a while since their owner has been gone (at work all day versus five minutes of taking out the trash). Some say it isn’t just memory involved here, though; that this is also due to an inner biological clock – that hormones, neural activity, and changes in body temperature are responsible, in much the same way they control an animal’s ability to know when it’s time to eat (McGrath, n.d.).

But cognitively, animals can still be conditioned to expect a reward at certain time intervals, which is exactly what Pavlov found in one of his famous experiments. By giving food to a dog at certain times, 30 minutes apart, the dog eventually began to salivate a few minutes before each future feeding (Roberts, 2002). Going back to my earlier mention of priming, this appears to be Hershey’s case as well.  My husband doesn’t always get home at the same time every night now like he used to, but the “magical hour” has long been ingrained in Hershey’s mind. When that time approaches, he knows. He’s ready. If he happens to be outside, he’ll start pacing, wanting to come inside so he can head to the door. And if he’s inside, that door is where he’ll be. Sitting, waiting…until once again, Daddy’s home!

References

Roberts, W. A. (2002). Are Animals Stuck in Time?, 128(3), 473–489. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.128.3.473

Do dogs understand the concept of time? : Dog Guide: Animal Planet. (n.d.). Retrieved March 15, 2014, from http://www.animalplanet.com/pets/do-dogs-understand-the-concept-of-time.htm

McGrath, J. (n.d.). How do dogs perceive time?: Animal Planet. Retrieved March 14, 2014, from http://www.animalplanet.com/pets/dogs-perceive-time.htm

Forgetful Me

Forgetful Me

Earl Culvey

 

Recently, I took an exam and felt I did well.  Unfortunately, my grade did not reflect that feeling of confidence.  Since returning to school after several years away I have always done well and maintained a respectable GPA.  I was a bit perplexed and distraught over this grade.  So I pondered on what may have changed.  I completed all readings and assignments, Check. I studied hard, Check. I followed directions, check.  Hmm, confounded.  Well, then it hit me.  Although I studied hard, I studied differently.  Normally, I do most of my studying at work on my downtime, which normally broke up my studying into short spurts.  Now, after starting a new job, I don’t have much time at work to study, so I began to “cram” late at night at home.  Sometimes, I spend five to six hours a night in the books.  And after some of our recent readings in Cognitive Psychology, it dawned on me, personal experience is a much better teacher than any book.  The spacing effect is a valuable psychological phenomenon for any student to be familiar with.

The spacing effect is termed after research showing that studying done in several short sessions is superior to studying in one long “cram” session.  (Goldstein, 2011)  The spacing effect has a long history of research and study in psychology, starting in Ebbinghaus’ classic memory studies in 1885.  (Susser and McCabe, 2013; Son and Simon, 2012)  Ebbinghaus’ first explanation says, “…with any considerable number of repetitions a suitable  distribution of them over a space of time is decidedly more advantageous than the massing of them at a single time.”  (1885) Since, 1885 many research studies have shown that given the same amount of time breaking your studying into multiple short periods is superior to massed learning.  (Susser and McCabe; 2012)

So, what is the mechanism for the spacing effect?  Son and Simon reviewed a number of studies and found several mechanisms and theories.  In reviewing a 1970 study by Bjork and Allen, Son and Simon stated the mechanism appeared to be better encoding at study trial 2 after an intervening task.  The intervening task was varied on its degree of  difficulty during the study and also showed some evidence for the later theory of desirable difficulty. (2012)  They defined desirable difficulty, “where the more difficult it is to retrieve the item at second trial (as a result of longer elapsed time since original study), the more easily that item would be retrieved again.”  (Son and Simon; 2012)  Another theory they discussed is the deficient-processing theory, which hypothesizes that during mass study we do not process the incoming stimuli thoroughly.  Otherwise, spaced study allows us to pay closer attention to the stimulus and hence better memory formation.  (Son and Simon; 2012)

What does this mean to you and me?  Since, research has proven that spaced study (study for short periods, multiple times) is superior to massed study (study for one long period) for long-term retention. (Son and Simon, 2012;  Susser and McCabe, 2013; Goldstein, 2011)  It would be beneficial for us to use this phenomenon to our advantage.  Susser and McCabe, reported a 2004 study that showed a moderate positive correlation between metacognitive  ability and cognitive strategy use.  Otherwise, stating “that individuals who are better at monitoring and adapting their strategies tend to utilize more deeper processing methods of learning.” (Susser and McCabe, 2013)  Or the better you are at adapting your study habits the better chance of  “deeper” learning and retention.

So, we have briefly discussed the long history of the spacing effect and some of its potential mechanisms.  We have only even barely scratched the surface of the research available and the numerous off-shoot topics connected to spaced study.  Research has shown the benefits of this phenomena to long-term retention.  I encourage you to further explore and use this phenomena to your advantage.  I have used this in the past, although admittedly, not intentionally.  When my study habits changed so did the outcome of those habits.  Again, adding some anecdotal evidence to the large volume of empirical evidence available.

 

 

 

 

 

References

Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das Gedchtnis. Untersuchungen zur experimentellen Psychologie. Leipzig: Duncker & Humblot; The English edition is Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). Memory. A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

Goldstein, E. (2011). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.

Son, L. K., & Simon, D. A. (2012). Distributed learning: Data, metacognition, and educational implications. Educational Psychology Review, 24(3), 379-399. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10648-012-9206-y

Susser, J. A., & McCabe, J. (2013). From the lab to the dorm room: Metacognitive awareness and use of spaced study. Instructional Science, 41(2), 345-363. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11251-012-9231-8

Forget Me Not: Effects of Alzheimer’s on Memory

By Sheryl Rivas

Memory is defined by our textbook “as the processes involved in retaining, retrieving, and using information about stimuli, images, events, ideas, and skills after the original information is no longer present.” ( Lesson 5) There is no specific area in our brain where memories are stored; our memories are encoded using different process involving almost all of the areas in our brain.

Memories “must be coded for by neurons in different areas of our brain.” (Lesson 8) The structure of a neuron contains the soma which is the cell body. The soma contains the nucleus of the neuron and is where dendrites are located. “Dendrites branch out from the soma and collect incoming signals and transmit them toward the soma.” (Lesson 2) The soma is connected to the axon, “the path in which neurons communicate with each other.” (Lesson 2) From the axon extend axon terminals which are branches that transmit signals “to other neurons or cells in the body” through synapses. (Lesson 2) A Synapse is a space found in between neurons, neurons are not connected to each other.

Action potentials are the foundation in which memories are formed. Signals are received in the dendrites from our environment, these signals cause action potentials. The signals travel from the dendrites to the soma, when the neuron reaches a threshold, action potentials fire from the soma down the axon and towards the axon terminals. The axon terminals communicate with other neurons or cells that ultimately carry out the action potentials, such as the coding of memories.

Alzheimer’s is defined by the Alzheimer’s association as “a type of dementia that causes problems with memory, thinking, and behavior.” (Alzheimer’s Association) Symptoms of Alzheimer’s include “difficulty remembering newly learned information, serious memory loss, confusion, disorientation, mood and behavior changes, and difficulty speaking, swallowing, and walking.”  These symptoms indicate damage to all the brain areas involved in the forming of memories. The occipital lobe is the area involved in processing visual information. The temporal lobe processes information we hear and is also involved in identification of objects and recognition. The parietal lobe receives sensory information which involves our sense of touch. Keep in mind that each lobe is involved in much more than I described, but for the sake of this blog I kept it simple and to the point. The symptoms in patients vary case by case and deficiencies do not occur in a specific order. The deficiencies specific to every individual is a marker for where the disease is taking place, and how extensively it is affecting every function in the brain.

Alzheimer’s disease prevents neurons in the brain from working properly causing problems in daily life activities. Neurons have many jobs; one of them is the making of proteins. In the making of proteins, neurotransmitters are the action potentials. In a healthy neurotransmitter, “an enzyme called alpha-secretase snips amyloid precursor protein (APP) releasing a fragment. A second enzyme, gama-secretase, also snips the APP in another place.” (NIH Senior Health) Proteins are the instruction manuals for the functions of the body; they control the release of chemicals in the brain that carry out functions. If these proteins are mutated in any way they can have negative effects on the body. In an unhealthy neurotransmitter, responsible for Alzheimer’s, the first cut in the protein is made by a different enzyme, beta-secretase. “Combined with the cut made by gamma-secretase, results in the release of short fragments of APP called beta-amyloid.” (NIH Senior Health) The fragments attach to the synapse of neurons and the continuous release of these fragments forms a beta amyloid plaque that prevents a neuron from working properly. These beta amyloid plaques grow larger the more fragments are released and eventually become insoluable, this causes the malfunction of neurons and is responsible for loss of cell function. Before they become insoluable, the fragments that are slowly building up at the synapse make the neuron less efficient causing memory problems, the beginning stages of Alzheimer’s.

In addition to the beta-amyloid plaques, another protein mutation in Alzheimer’s causes neurofibrillary tangles. “Neurofibrillary tangles are made when a protein called tau is modified. In normal brain cells, tau stabilizes structures critical to the cell’s internal transport system.” (NIH Senior Health) The internal transport system refers to the axon of a neuron, which carry out signals to the axon terminals. The modification to tau causes the internal transport system to fall apart, stopping the transmission of signals from one neuron to another and ultimately kills the cell. When the cells die and disconnect from each other they cause the brain to shrink and lose function. At this point, we would see the more advance effects of Alzheimer’s in a person such as speech impairment, loss of mobility and the inability to complete activities of daily living.

There are current drug trials going on that show great potential on the cure and prevention of Alzheimer’s. They key is to diagnose the disease as early as possible, because when it is in the advance stages the cells have begun to die and cannot be recovered. One specific drug trial is attempting to target the disease through the immune system. The immune system naturally tries to remove plaque buildup at the synapses of cells but it does not complete it efficiently. The drug is intended to work like a vaccine, making the immune system stronger and more able to fight disease efficiently. Trial results on mice have shown promising results with the successful removal of plaque buildup in the brain. These results are an indication that we are a step closer to curing a disease that has claimed the lives of many individuals throughout the years.

 

References

Alzheimer’s Disease & Dementia | Alzheimer’s Association. (n.d.). Alzheimer’s Disease & Dementia | Alzheimer’s Association. Retrieved March 9, 2014, from http://www.alz.org/alzheimers_disease_what_is_alzheimers.asp

Inside the Brain: An Interactive Tour | Alzheimer’s Association. (n.d.). Inside the Brain: An Interactive Tour | Alzheimer’s Association. Retrieved March 9, 2014, from http://www.alz.org/alzheimers_disease_4719.asp

Inside the Brain: Unraveling the Mystery of Alzheimer’s Disease. (n.d.). Inside the Brain: Unraveling the Mystery of Alzheimer’s Disease. Retrieved March 9, 2014, from http://nihseniorhealth.gov/alzheimersdisease/whatisalzheimersdisease/video/a2_na.html?intro=yes

Lesson 2 : Cognitive Neuroscience. (n.d.). PSYCH256: Introduction to Cognitive Psychology. Retrieved March 9, 2014, from https://courses.worldcampus.psu.edu/sp14/psych256/002/content/03_lesson/printlesson.html

Lesson 5 : Short-term & Working Memory. (n.d.). PSYCH256: Introduction to Cognitive Psychology. Retrieved March 9, 2014, from https://courses.worldcampus.psu.edu/sp14/psych256/002/content/06_lesson/printlesson.html

Lesson 8 : Long-term Memory: Encoding & Retrieval . (n.d.). PSYCH256: Introduction to Cognitive Psychology. Retrieved March 12, 2014, from https://courses.worldcampus.psu.edu/sp14/psych256/002/content/09_lesson/printlesson.html

 

 

Two Cups of Hot Cocoa a Day May Help Keep the Doctor Away

                                                                                                                                shutterstock-coffee

With all of the cold weather we’ve been facing recently, I’ve found that there really is nothing better than curling up on the couch with a good book and a cup of hot cocoa. Whipped cream, marshmallows, cinnamon; no matter how you enjoy it, hot cocoa is a great way to keep warm and de-stress on a chilly winter evening. But, did you know that it is also good for your brain?

A recent study done at Harvard Medical School shows that there may be a connection between the antioxidant flavanol found in hot chocolate and healthy circulation in the working areas of the brain. Although more research is needed, they believe cognitive function, and therefore memory, can be improved by regular cocoa consumption in elderly patients with vascular damage. The scientists determined the effects of the hot cocoa by measuring performance on cognitive exams as well as neurovascular coupling.

Neurovascular coupling is the relationship between neural activity and cerebral blood flow. An increase in neural activity leads to an increase in blood flow to that part of the brain. As we get older, blood flow decreases which contributes to a decline in cognitive functions. Poor vascular health is a risk factor for dementia in the elderly.

Cocoa has been determined several times before to improve circulation and lower blood pressure, but this is the first study showing how improved vascular health can also help to prevent memory decline as we age. The scientists admit that much more work is needed in order to prove the link between cocoa, vascular health, and cognitive functioning; but, wouldn’t it be nice if the only thing standing between you and a better memory was a daily delicious cup of hot cocoa?

 

References:

Hurwitz, S., Salat, D.H., Sorond, F.A., & et al. (2013, August 7). “Neurovascular coupling, cerebral white matter integrity, and response to cocoa in older people.” Neurology. Retrieved from http://www.neurology.org/content/early/2013/08/07/WNL.0b013e3182a351aa.full.html.

Paddock, C. (2013, August 8). “Hot chocolate may prevent memory decline.” Medical News Today. Retrieved from http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/264551.

 

Remembering Dreams

Why is it that sometimes we are able to wake from a dream and remember it exactly but throughout the day we find it harder and harder to remember the information in the dream? Not only do we find it more difficult to remember the dream, but sometimes by the end of the day we are only left with only an impression of the dream. Maybe the reason could be that the process of remembering semantic meanings in memory is better for recalling information than episodic memories enables us to retrieve the overall impression we have of the dream. Maybe it is possible that we interpret our dreams into a semantic meaning, enabling us to relate the experience to what we already have stored in our long term memory. By capturing the dream’s semantic meaning instead of the precise detail we are able to associate it with things we already know thus making it easier for the dream to be stored in our long-term memory. Either way, the focus of this paper is more on why we are able to remember some dreams, unable to remember others and only remember parts of some.
Explanations as to why we easily forget our dreams have been numerous over the decades. Several researches conducted in the area of dream recall have suggested that because we have a limited capacity for short term memory distractions and interferences that occur immediately after we wake up are in competition for that limited capacity. Because we are unable to intake so much information into our short-term memory the dreams are replaced by these other stimuli that are competing for our memory space. Our dreams are therefore forgotten and replaced by these wakeful stimuli (Koulack and Goodenough, 1976). That sounds like a pretty reasonable assessment considering that we do know that the short term memory has a capacity for between five to seven items that can be held for 15- 30 seconds until they are transferred into long-term memory by processes such as rehearsal, encoding, and chunking (Goldstein, 2011,pgs 118-126). So, why then, are we able to remember some of our dreams?
Consolidation is the process that converts fragile memories that can be distributed to a more permanent state to where they are not so easily disrupted (Goldstein, 2011, pg. 193). So if I dream something at 9 o’clock at night maybe my brain is imploring chunking to remember that dream and then later consolidate it over the hippocampus where I am able to connect it through other parts of my brain without the use of the hippocampus and thus making it a part of my long term memory. This is possible but for most of us our dreams are forgotten over time, at least bits and pieces. Perhaps, while dreaming, I am able to form connections in a deeper, semantic meaning of the overall experience of my dreams and therefore connect it with what is previously stored in my long- term memory. Maybe this accelerated the memory process where my hippocampus is not needed at all to remember the dreams because I have at some level already stored information that connects the pieces of the dream together to form a memory. So instead of me actually remembering my dream I am remembering the overall semantic meaning and “filling in the gaps” as they make sense to me, a theory relatively close to what Freud’s theory of dreaming was; that is the analytical approach (Freud, 1965).
Another idea as to why we may be able to remember some of our dreams and not others may be influenced by primacy and recency effects. Primacy effects being the ability to remember stimuli at the beginning of a sequence and possibly rehearsing it in order to transfer them into long- term memory (Goldstein, 2011, pg. 151). The ability to experience these dreams earlier on and then rehearse them gives reason to believe that they are only the ones stored in long term memory and therefore are remembered. Recency effects are those stimuli that are presented last in a list and are most recent in short term memory (Goldstein, 2011, pg.153). Research has found that dreams sometimes are easier to recall if had earlier in the night and sometimes easier to recall when experienced in the morning so long as numerous dreams were not reported (Meier et, al, 1968).
There are many different possibilities to why some of us are able to access our dreams, why some of us are able to access some of our dreams, why some of us are unable to access any of our dreams, and why some of us experience a combination of the above. Whatever the definitive answer, research as well as experience has shown us we have a long way to go in understanding this area of the mind as well as human experience.

References:
Freud, S. (1965). The Interpretation of Dreams. The Hearst Corporation. Avon Books
Goldstein, B. (2011). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 3rd Edition. Wadsworth, Inc.
Koulack, D., & Goodenough, D. R. (1976). Dream recall and dream recall failure: An arousal-retrieval model. Psychological Bulletin, 83(5), 975-984. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.83.5.975
Meier, C., Ruef, H., Ziegler, A., and Hall, C. (1968) Forgetting of Dreams in the Laboratory. Perceptual and Motor Skills: Volume 26, Issue, pp. 551-557. doi: 10.2466/pms.1968.26.2.551

Dementia

Dementia is a chronic disorder in the brain that can be diagnosed with symptoms of memory loss, personality changes and impaired reasoning. This can be caused from an injury or a brain disease, as well as simply old age. It is similar to Alzheimer’s but not quite as life-threatening. With Alzheimer’s, patients will survive but there is no cure, and chances are the disease will take their life, but that hasn’t been shown for Dementia patients. This disease, at the appropriate stage, requires full-time care and leaves people feeling empty.

In the article that I read, it discusses that Dementia is becoming more and more of an issue in today’s economy and people aren’t really aware of it. They want to increase funding for research and set a goal to have a cure by 2025. Health Secretary, Jeremy Hunt, said that “there is too little funding going into research.” Hunt also said, “It’s a big, big ambition to have. If we don’t aim for the stars we won’t land on the moon,” regarding the goal of 2025 to have a cure. The concern is that by 2050, 135 million people will be suffering from the disease which could be a huge downfall to individual’s lives.

I think the fact someone is trying to do something about the disease is awesome. My grandmother suffers from Dementia, and when they say it affects the whole family, it does. I’ve watched her personality change so much over the past 5 years. From happy-go-lucky to confused and mean. She asks a lot of questions, a lot of the same questions, often. She’ll forget the answer to the question she had just asked, therefore she needs to ask it again. She is very defensive in most cases. If you tell her that she is wrong, she gets very angry at you and sternly tells you that she is not wrong.

It’s coming to the point where we don’t want to leave her alone in her home because she doesn’t take care of herself. She’ll forget to eat unless someone is there to tell her to. In the rare occasion that she does decide to eat, we don’t want her to leave the over or stove on and then cause a fire. There are so many precautions that we are taking. My mom mom has 11 children, so they’ve created a schedule for her so someone is with her in the afternoon and night. So far it is working fairly well. She is picking up on it, which we didn’t want but I think she may be starting to realize that it’s necessary to have help and the extra company is always nice.

This article just really makes me happy, as someone in the economy, and as a granddaughter. Dementia is a serious disease, and not many people know about it I feel like. I think it’s great that people are finally coming together to help fight it. Unfortunately, this cure will not help my grandmother, but if it succeeds, it will help so many others and for that I am grateful.

 

Sources:

http://www.bbc.com/news/health-25318194

https://www.google.com/#q=dementia+definition