31
Oct 10

Week 11 Readings…

            This week’s readings definitely provided some clarity for me in terms of the theoretical framework. I have spent the past week struggling to synthesize all of the readings that we have completed into finite and concise summaries, and these readings presented me with a glimpse of how I can do that for myself.

            The article by Blumenfeld provided me with the basic overview of the theories (including their limitations) that I needed at this point in my struggle to gather my thoughts regarding the different theories we have discussed this semester. Blumenfeld also presented concrete ways in which researchers have tried implanting situated instruction into the classroom. Because I am relatively new to reading about educational research, I had never heard about the various programs currently being employed in the classroom.

I have struggled over the past couple months with trying to find exact implications for these theories in classroom practice. Greeno even recognized that putting theory into practice within the classroom may present more similarities between the theories than seen in strictly theoretical discussions:“In a situative study, individual cognition is considered in relation to more general patterns of interaction” (p.84). When discussing various aspects of situated research, Greeno states:”The goal is to understand cognition as the interaction among participants and tools in the context of an activity. For this reason, it is often said that the situated perspectived studies distributed cognition,” (p.84). Again, Greeno recognizes that situated studies may include an examination of the cognitive approach to learning. I find myself struggling with this point. Maybe the situative and cognitive perspectives aren’t two exclusive entities, as I previously believed. Are the theories truly on opposite side of the spectrum, or is the work of Greeno just somewhere more towards the center of the spectrum than the other situated theorists that we’ve read about this semester?

            Blumenfeld also recognized the struggle that teachers often face when implying new techniques in the classroom. Through the discussion of project-based learning, Blumenfeld discussed that it took the teachers several years to be able to apply this proves of teaching to their classrooms. I think that the most interesting point in this discussion was when Blumenfeld recognized that teachers often take an approach and adapt it to fit their unique classrooms. This is something that I have known teachers to do, however, we have not discussed this within the context of these theoretical frameworks. Again, I return to the point that I have made many times before: I think that I read these frameworks and looked for examples of how they could fit directly into a classroom without recognizing the opportunity for teacher adaptation. Could this be the reason why I have struggled to really understand the influences of these different theories on the school system?

            The diSessa reading functioned as mainly a summary of the cognitive perspective for me. DiSessa made me think about misconceptions in terms of their positive and negative influences on learning, as opposed to just a discussion of what misconceptions are. Misconceptions may not be as detrimental to student learning as I sometimes think. As long as the teacher approaches the misconceptions in a manner that can provoke discussion and alter them, they may function as a successful learning tool in the classroom. Is this a difficult task to accomplish? I believe so, but if done correctly, I think that it can be very beneficial. 

 

 


31
Oct 10

Week 11

            After this week’s readings I feel as though I have a better understanding of the preceding theories that theoretical frameworks have developed in response to.  I enjoyed the Blumenfeld et al. article, Chapter 4: Teaching for Understanding, because it gave a thorough synopsis of many of the concepts we have discussed in class over the last few weeks. Blumenfeld et al. discussed the branches of constructivism, and although they are very different I was able to pick up on how they can relate.   On page 830-831 a quotation from Bruer demonstrated a bridge between the cognitive and sociocultural theories that I had trouble connecting in the past.  It states,  “The significance of sociocultural context, communities and discourse for cognition and learning…emerged in part because researchers have attempted to apply cognitive science to authentic classroom tasks rather than just artificial laboratory tasks..focused on individual problem solvers… when researchers brought ideas about domain specific trajectories, active learning, and metacognition to the classroom they realized that other factors influenced learning as well…” (Bruer 1995).  Although I am still trying to find my own grounding of what I deem to by my definition of how learning takes place, I do know that I believe that it is a mix of the cognitive and sociocultural activities that allow for the process to occur.  The programs that were designed as examples of the constructivist theories were interesting applications to what the theoretical framework provided.  In particular, the Project-Based Science caught my eye.  It seems like a tactic that provides reason and drive to doing an activity. I could see projects and problems that are found in the real world, to be something that could motivate the students to become a part of a scientific community, rather than just passively learning through an independent approach.  The goal of Blumenfeld et al. was to find ways to make the theories into practical techniques in the classroom, but the most important thing is to gain the support of current and future teachers.  As in all professions, teachers need to be developing themselves and changing how they teach their class in order to find the way that learning occurs best. 

            diSessa’s A History of Conceptual Change Research pointed out that the term “concept” is something that is so loosely defined by theorists, even though it is crucial to what entails a misconception.  As quoted on page 269, Toulmin stated, “The term concept is one that everybody uses and nobody explains – still less defines.” In general I feel like I find myself struggling to understand what the theorists are implying based off of the terminology that they use to convey their theory of learning. I did like that this reading also touched base with other readings that we have read earlier in the semester.  I found it interesting that Posner et al.‘s theory was not meant to be something for instruction.  I guess when I first read that article I assumed it was something that should be automatically translated into the classroom, while it was quite different- it was something that simply supported a theoretical framework. 

            Lastly, Greeno’s Learning in Activity also provided a lot of background on the development of theories.  I was confused in particular on page 82 of the reading, under the subtitle- “Including Interaction in Cognitive Analysis.” Was Greeno implying that due to the way experimentation takes place it is impossible to find the activity and tools of an individual through experimentation, and rather this is why the activity system needs to be looked at?  I don’t know why, but I have trouble understanding Greeno’s point here.  If he is saying that people can’t be studied because the social context changes, what good would any experiment have then in his point of view?

 


31
Oct 10

Blumenfeld, Greeno, diSessa, and Hewson

I found this week’s articles helpful in light of my first attempt at my theoretical framework of learning.  Though I didn’t find much new, as already mentioned by Dr. McD, reading some of the “summaries” did help clarify a few points (at least a little):

1)      Issues with transfer and generalizability (including assessment):

The chapter by Blumenfeld et al., mentioned some of the difficulties in implementing project based curriculum in the classroom.  A substantial difficulty was the transfer of ability to implement these types of programs from researcher to teacher.  The teachers had significant problems in “adopting” new methods and took about three years to incorporate, to the researchers satisfaction, problem based curricula in the classroom (Blumenfeld,et al. , 1997).  From a situated standpoint, these difficulties would be expected because the teachers were entering into a new way of practice which is a process rather than an outcome.   Also, teachers are learners too, and oftentimes they are expected to learn in ways deemed “unsuitable” for students.  Blumenfeld et al writes:

What is noteworthy about recent constructivist approaches as compared with older transmission models and early transformation models is that there are no prescriptions for instruction. That is, newer constructivist-based approaches are strongly contextualized. Since local conditions are so important, all one can expect from the theory is a set of principles to help guide teachers as they create practices that are congruent with constructivist theory. [p. 833].

Blumenfeld et al., also discuss how, using a situated framework, assessments cannot be standardized but instead should be tied to student interaction, student performance and/or artifact production.

2)      Issues with the link between cognitive learning theories and situated learning theories.

Both Blumenfeld and Greeno restate what we have read before:  that “subscribing” to one theory does not exclude the interactions that are the focus of the other.   Also, regardless of what theory is used to describe the phenomena, learning applications may be the same (I’m still trying to wrap my tiny mind around that one 🙂 )  Greeno 2006 writes, “In a situative study, individual cognition is considered in relation to more general patterns of interaction.”[p.84]  Greeno even proposes “There is nothing in the situative perspective that precludes analyses on multiple levels simultaneously.” [p. 84]  These multiple levels include cognition.  Greeno reiterates that, though individual cognition is important, it is not the focus of situated theory stating:

Situative researchers generally do not assume that our models of these conventions, practices, and identities necessarily correspond to cognitive representations inside participant’s minds. Of course, people often do construct internal representations of these conventions and practices, and these constructions and interpretations are critical in activity and are an important topic for the learning sciences [p. 89]

One conclusion by Greeno did throw me for a loop however: “The situative perspective is a synthesis of the two major scientific approaches to understanding human behavior: cognitive science and interactional studies.” [p. 92]  I can see how situated and cognitive theories are not totally incompatible with each other, but I hadn’t  thought of the “situate perspective” being an incorporation of cognitive science and interactional studies.

As far as the conceptual change readings are concerned; they were helpful in summarizing what we have previously read on conceptual change, but I didn’t feel like they will be useful in my theoretical framework since I am focusing more on the situated perspective than cognitive theory.  I’m thoroughly confused however, on diSessa’s breakdown on the different theories behind conceptual change (e.g. coherence vs. theory theory vs. knowledge in pieces), though I admit I didn’t put in the time to really think about what the distinction was between them.  Maybe this week’s blog discussion and a possible re-read will help clarify.


31
Oct 10

Week 11 (insert witty title here)

diSessa – A History of Conceptual Change Research

diSessa’s article focused on conceptual change and how it relates to science and learning.  One factor that diSessa discussed was the idea of misconceptions.  These can be minor misconceptions that can be easily corrected or they may require a more in depth explanation or work in order to correct them.  The example that she referred to throughout the article that involved drawing a force diagram for a simple physics experiment was a clear example of the misconceptions that are present within our classrooms.  The quote that diSessa included from learner.org, “even the brightest students in the class [have] false ideas based on enduring misconceptions that traditional instructional methods cannot overcome.” (2006, p. 269) is a simple way to state what is happening in our classrooms.  It is not that students are coming to us as a completely empty slate, they are coming to us with ideas about the subject matter even if they have not has a lot of experience with our class work, they have certainly heard something about it from a friend, family member, television or the movies.  The incommensurability that Kuhn talked about is a way to classify their resulting conceptual changes based on learning new information.  This idea of changing beliefs certainly tied in with the Posner article (which was also cited in the paper) of the four ‘steps’ necessary for conceptual change to occur.


Greeno – Learning in Activity

One difference that Greeno mentions as a difference in the way cognitive and situated research is designed was the additional factors that were taken into consideration.  In this case, I feel that I side more with Greeno and the situated frame of thought.  Greeno (2006) states that the perspective that the situated school of thought is using is the idea of distributed cognition which is defined as being “problem solving, planning, and reasoning are accomplished by a group of people, working together with complex technological artifacts and with material representations they generate during the task (diagrams, figures, and models)” (p. 85).    This is an idea that I agree with; the situated perspective analyzes multiple levels of interaction to fully determine an individual’s learning.  There is not a clear way to remove the factors of the environment completing from a specific situation, therefore, these are contributing factors that must be taken into consideration.  In fact, Greeno goes on to quote Pickering (1995) and describes it as a “dance of agency, involving material agency, disciplinary agency, and conceptual agency” (p. 88).  For me this shows the balance that must be present in order for everything to work out.  The situations are not simply ‘thrown together’ but they are choreographed and are interacting with each other to produce learning in individuals.


Blumenfeld, et al. – Teaching for Understanding (1997)

            The first part of this article that discussed process-product research really caught my attention.  Within this second several thoughts came to mind as I was reading.  The first was that this seems to be the way that the LA teachers were assessed in their effectiveness.  It mentions examining test scores and relating it to the differences in teachers.  This discussion was evident was presented in a 1986 report by Brophy and Good.  The results they were looking at were very similar to those that we saw relating to the LA teachers.  Blumenfeld et al. also mention some limitations to this and one of them being that it only identifies the effective or ineffective teachers, it does not identify those who are exemplary (p. 823).  It also did not take into consideration the context and subject matter differences.  I am now wondering if the LA teachers had these areas addressed differently when their results were calculated.  The other information that I found interesting in this section is that this is the model that my school uses for teaching.  The four steps that are mentioned (opening, development, practice, assignment of about 15 minutes) is the same set of information that I was given and told that my lesson plans needed to include.  I was surprised to see that it was developed in 1983. 


28
Oct 10

Assignments and Such

First, I have posted the samples of theoretical frameworks and I have posted the theoretical readings for the first assignment. The empirical readings will be up soon.

Second, this is a talk by Sir Ken Robinson that has been turned (cleverly, I think) into an animation. It is a talk and critique of schools and schooling with emphasis on the impact on creativity. Worth a look if you have ten minutes to spare.
See you in two weeks.

27
Oct 10

Thinking like an octopus | Harvard Gazette

http://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2010/10/thinking-like-an-octopus/

Thought this might be interesting in our ongoing conversation on crows, monkeys, seagulls and other forms of animal intelligence. I like the part about how things learned via different eyes does not transfer, brings a whole new meaning to situated.


26
Oct 10

A Murder of Crows: Nature special on crow learning

Hi All… Thought you might be interested since the topic of crow intelligence has popped up once or twice in class.  🙂  This is a PBS Nature episode involving a study on how crows learn and if learning by parents is passed on to the next generation.  Oh, by the way, in case you didn’t know (which I didn’t), a group of crows is called a “Murder.”

You can watch the full episode here:
http://www.pbs.org/wnet/nature/episodes/a-murder-of-crows/full-episode/5977/


25
Oct 10

Questions for your theoretical framework

Here are some questions that you may want to think about as you write up your drafts of your theoretical framework:

1. How do the different theories (groups of theories) deal with context and which approach to context do I find more compelling and well supported/argued?
2. How do the different theories (groups of theories) describe learning and knowing? How do they describe it and/or measure it? 
3. What implications for teaching do the different theories claim? Which do you feel best represents an approach to teaching you support?
4. What are areas that you feel you don’t understand well or feel has not been dealt with well by any of the authors you have read? What questions remain for you? Are there places where you expected authors to address something and the glossed over it or avoided it entirely?
Overall, remember that you are writing a description of your understanding of teaching and learning using the readings from the class as theoretical support for your point of view. There is not a correct answer to this, and you may have a hybrid view on these things drawing on multiple theories.

21
Oct 10

Lessons from Japan

A couple of weeks ago, our assistant principal sent the faculty this video to watch.  It is an interesting comparison of our schools/performance with Japan’s.  I thought it fit will with last night’s class discussion.  It is worth the 3 minutes it takes to watch it.

 

http://www.cbsnews.com/video/watch/?id=6912924n&tag=related;photovideo


20
Oct 10

Science Education Articles

So I wrote this blog twice, and the first time I accidentally ended up deleted the post because I was trying to find a way to edit it. The second time I tried to post it and my penn state account had timed out, so my blog didn’t get posted. Anyway, I’m hoping that third time is the charm. 

I really thought that the ideas from Roth and Eijck’s article were interesting. Their aspirations for what learning should be preparing us for align with my thoughts on what the goals of education should be. I liked their idea about preparing students to gain knowledgeability, or the ability to seek knowledge, build upon it, and use it. Also, I liked their idea about making a student a d�brouillard/e, or someone who can think on their feet and adapt to the environment around them to solve problems. So often, I think that these are the most important skills to learn in school, and I don’t believe they are even being touched at the undergraduate level. Roth and Eijck talk about how a student needs to have an desire to seek knowledge, a desire to find answers to questions and problems. I feel that the current twelve years of education teaches students how to cram facts into their head for the test, forget about them, and move on. It is more about how to complete requirements than about wanting to complete a task to the best of one’s ability. 

I liked the author’s point about learning on a social level. Schooling has become largely a competition among students rather than a platform to promote social skills. Too often, students are required to work alone and get assessed alone. School is more about out-performing the student next to you and less about discovering how helping each other can produce a better outcome. Synergism, is not often taken advantage of. In my experience, “group work” was often the “smartest” kids completing the test, while the other students became confused with the activity. I think, too often, in classrooms group work often becomes a process of clique forming. Students work with students with whom they want to work rather than a diverse group like society is. I’ve gone through countless undergraduate classes not even knowing some of the names of students around me, students that I sit next to for fifteen weeks straight. To me, the most important reform in school needs to promote students working with every one of their peers. 

I wish Roth and Eijck would have given more examples of how they see their ideas manifesting in a school setting. I would guess that they wouldn’t see these ideas being applied to the current “school” setting. I liked their examples of having students “growing green spaces, seeding parks with butterfly larvae, hatching runs of salmon to repopulate streams in which the fish are extinct, contributing to the environmental health of their community.” These all sound like very interesting and educational activities with a variety of “teachable moments.” To have a school that has students go out into the world and do these activities sounds very Utopian. I do not see how these ideas would ever be able to be applied to our society’s current view on schooling. I also do not know how teachers would fit into this picture. The only thing I do know, is that I like the picture and wish that it could be a reality. 

Skip to toolbar