17
Oct 10

JRST articles

Well all three articles stressed to social nature of meaning-making.  But they all had their own takes on it.

Chin & Osborne had the strongest paper, at least for me, maybe because they seemed to incorporate both the conceptual change and the situational perspectives.  For example, they write on page 884 ” Both sociocognitive conflict … and cognitive elaboration have been proposed to explain why collaborative discourse and argumentation may foster conceptual learning in socially mediated learning.”  That idea of cognitive dissonance from Posner seems to be here. As a practioner, the idea of scaffolding the social interactions with the argument diagram is a needed one – students need to be taught how to learn from one another in the way that scientists learn from one another.  That representation in another tool for students to communicate their thoughts – I guess I could say the diagram mediated the explanations of the students (?).

 Varelas et al. was interesting because of the varieties of the communities of practice in which the students were involved.  They were in a school, in a science class, but also practicing the dramatic arts.  Interesting confluence.  Taken to the extreme, if knowledge is purely situative and does not transfer across contexts at all,  then does that mean that these students may only be able to access their knowledge  while performing in front of others on stage?   

All three articles emphasized the students use of language as key to their learning.  And it was not language filled with scientific vocabulary.  The students expressed their ideas in their vernacular.  Also, the students talked extensively, not just a short response to a teacher question.  This is key to promoting a socially oriented classroom.  Language itself is not enough.  The students must find the language to express their ideas or concerns in a way that others can understand.  Also, the language mediates the data gathered by the researchers.  They are interpreting the language from their own perspective.

Finally, I now want to read a recent,pure ( or close to it) ” in the head” paper to compare what it looks like in the  real life of ed. research rather than from a more philosophical viewpoint.  I cannot believe that I just called educational research “real life ”  – I have been assimilated!


17
Oct 10

The Benefits of Group Discussions

After reading our three articles from the Journal of Research in Science Teaching, I have a new found appreciation for how our class is run. The big idea that I got from looking at these three articles collectively was that students derive much benefit from being able to work in groups and discuss concepts with each other. The Chin and Osbourne article was particularly illustrative (is that a word?) to me. I was struck by the description of how the teacher intervention in the “worst” group actually hindered their discussion and might have prevented them from coming to a better understanding of the concepts (pgs 900-901). I thought that while the example was good and showed that students are capable of coming to a higher understanding on their own terms, it wasn’t an example that should taken as proof that teachers should leave students to themselves. I think a certain amount of teacher intervention can be good, although apparently not so much under the specific circumstances of the study in the article.

The Radinsky, et. al. article also demonstrated the power of student discussion.  Through the subtle nudging of the teacher and the questions of the other students, “Camilla” was able to more clearly explain and support her theory about why the days are shorter in fall and winter. I was a fan of the whole set-up the teacher created, through the journals about students initial ideas of the topic, to the explanation of their theory to the rest of  the class and the ensuing explanation. I found the authors’ breakdown of the discourse moves (pg 634) to be pretty accurate of how articles are actually constructed.

The final article by Varelas, et. al. I found to be a subtle argument in favor of student group discussion. While not the main point of the article, I found that in the various excerpts there was evidence of students gaining a better understanding of concepts through their acting and through the teachers asking questions about how the molecules behave or the chains of the food web. I liked the idea of acting out science, although its probably not something that would work well once students got to a certain age.


17
Oct 10

Journal of Research in Science Teaching

            The three articles were published throughout the current year in the Journal of Research in Science Teaching.  All three of these articles were case studies that in one shape or form had grounding in the construction of meaning through social situations.  First of all I would like to say that it was really neat to be able to read about the set up of the research experiments.  In the future semesters I will be looking to do my own research, and reading about the methods of other researchers was an application that was interesting to receive insight into. 

            Radinsky et al. made the point that the possession of science knowledge by the individual rather than constructed socially is a belief that is stressed and embedded in the current science education culture and standards.  This accepted practice is something that is leading to a huge division between the nature of science in a scientific community, and how students learn within a classroom. Radinsky et al. stated on page 619, in reference to what their research entails, “It emphasizes that professional communities of scientists generate new knowledge through a collective, contested, negotiated process, based on communication and mutual accommodation of ideas, rather than simply through the individual exercise of abstract logical reasoning.” The  Vygotsky-like philosophy that Radinsky et al. brought forward in their research provided students with opportunities to interact and co-construct a shared meaning for a phenomenon.  I believe that this interaction allowed the students to unite with the science learning process in a noteworthy and meaningful manner.  Camila, and the students of the classroom, underwent lesson plans that challenged them to construct a socially accepted theory, and talk themselves through misconceptions in order to arrive at an understanding that was viable to what the scientific world would deem appropriate.  I thought it was really neat to read the discourse of the students, and how the interactions between the students altered the theory and conceptions of the class.  The part of the study that grabbed my attention the most was when Camila was challenged to explain her logic with greater detail, and eventually arrived at a notion of a “wrapping effect.”  Through the questions of Melinda and the teacher, Camila developed her theory by the support of others.  There was a cognitive approach that Camila took to create the  meaning that she derived, but she was able to be influenced and translate this meaning by a social aspect of learning.  Although this process in Radinsky et al. takes longer than simply telling the students what the answer was,  I believe there is no comparison between the two ways to approach this situation.  The thought process helped the students arrive at the correct scientific explanation, while still holding true to the nature of science and the fashion that scientists use to discover new findings.  I was also impressed with this study because of the acknowledgment of developing a learning environment that allowed for the student who is usually perceived as low achieving, to be able to relate to the material and make connections that are stronger than what would occur during a plain old lesson.  When I finished reading this article I was content to have an example of a socially constructed meaning in the classroom, where the researchers do not knock the importance of the cognitive approach.  Radinksy et al. rather demonstrated how the social discourse in the classroom allowed for learning that related to scientific practices. 

            Chin et al. was another group that demonstrated the importance of social activity.  For Chin et al. the social activity that was particularly important was argumentation.  As stated on page 883, “it has been argued that, if our students are to be enculturated into the workings of the scientific enterprise, argumentation should be a core component of school science.”  I found many of the practices Chin et al. did really interesting.  The practice of constructing question webs to organize a structure to the students thought process, and using a scaffolding method to allow students to take more responsibility for a successful argumentation process, were two research methods I thought were very interesting.  The ability to use evidence and create counterarguments are two skills that I think are very important to translate into constructive classroom discussions.  I thought that this article presented a very good case for the importance of teaching students the art of argumentation that is constructive to help build common understandings.

            The only article that I had trouble relating to was Varelas et al.  Overall, I understand the key point that drama in a science class could play on the emotions and feelings of the student, and that shared meanings may be created by this process.  I just don’t know what type of function this could serve when teaching middle school or high school students.  I do like the thought that drama can spark more interest from the students, but I don’t know how it could translate to a lot of the curriculum that is to be taught throughout the year.  I think that this study did not interest me as much because it was an elementary cohort that was studied in the article. 

           


17
Oct 10

JRST – Learning through Experience

            At the start of this week’s readings, I wasn’t quite sure how (or even if) all of the readings would examine a similar topic. While thinking about the readings after completing them, I found that each paper was unique in content discussed; however, all of them explicitly commented on the importance of group interactions in the learning process. It was fascinating to see how these researchers took the theories that we have been discussing in class (such as those of Greeno and Vygotsky) and used them to support their experiments and analyze their data. It was also very enjoyable to read about how some aspects of these theories can be applied specifically to a science classroom. I’ve struggled with trying to apply these theoretical frameworks to classroom situations over the past seven weeks, and I now see that this was likely because I was trying to find a situation that encompassed all aspect of each theory.

            One point that I believe each researcher made, either implicitly or explicitly, was that language is an extremely important aspect in learning. When discussing Vygotsky’s theory in class several weeks ago, we spent time discussing the significance of developing a common language to enable different members of the same culture to communicate their ideas. The article by Radinsky et al. explicitly emphasized the significance of language: “The science talks were used for making students’ private thoughts public, providing a space in which to think things through and clarify understandings” (p 623). These authors utilized language in their study and found that developing common understandings of words through scientific talk helped the students to developed more well-rounded understandings of the information. Chin and Osborne also encouraged the students to develop a common language through questioning each other and responding to such questions with their interpretations. This argumentation provided the students with a social opportunity in which they were able to further their understandings of scientific material. All three of the articles illustrated the significance of understanding a common scientific language in the process of learning through social interactions.
            I found the article by Varelas et al. to be particularly interesting. I had never heard about the use of dramatizations in the learning of scientific concepts, and after reading the title, I wasn’t sure if I would see the significance of using this idea in the classroom. Varelas showed that through student participation in drama activities, they were able to “[negotiate] ambiguity and [re-articulate] understandings, thus marking this embodied meaning making as a powerful way of their engagement with science” (p. 321). It appears that the students were able to increase their understanding of topics such as matter through engaging in drama activities. The students were able to learn the information through their social interactions with each other, illustrating the significance that these interactions have on student learning. I’m not sure if I see the effectiveness of utilizing such a technique in a high school classroom. The article examined the effectiveness with students of elementary school age; I would be interested to see if high school students could learn scientific concepts through dramatizations as well.

            Based on the titles of the articles and their provided abstracts, I was under the impression that they would all deal solely with the social aspect of learning. However, this was not the case. The article by Ratinsky et al. explicitly mentioned the importance of using both social and cognitive theories within the classroom. All of the articles recognized that learning occurs on an individual basis, as well as in group situations. As I attempt to figure out where I stand on the spectrum of situated and cognitive, I found it refreshing to see that some researchers identify the various roles that each theory plays in learning. Learning does not solely occur in the cognitive sense, and it does not solely occur in social situations. I’m still grappling with my own interpretations of each theory, but it’s nice to see the applicability of certain aspects to the classroom environment. 


17
Oct 10

SCIED – That is one Juicy Question!

 

The readings for this week were quite refreshing, as they took me out of the classroom (and psychology laboratory, in some of the articles we have read) and into informal learning environments. I enjoyed all three reading, but found that the article by Roth and Van Eijck (2010) to be the least enjoyable.

The article by Gutwill and Allen (2009) argued that “designed informal settings constitute ideal environments for teaching and learning inquiry skills…features often unavailable in schools (p. 711). Research of informal settings such as science museums, by other researchers, is used to further support their claim. The authors defined terms such as “proposing actions” and “interpreting results” as constituting museum visitors’ inquiry skills (p. 713). A study was composed that compared two different “games” based strategies and two controls, differing in terms of kinds of mediation and guidance participants received. It would appear that Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory of learning is supported by the authors, in that mediation and social interaction are both key to the research design. Results from this study pointed to the fact that the game, called the Juicy Question, was the most effective at engaging learners and producing new knowledge, which was later recalled. A critique of mine, which was also expressed by the authors, was that of the activity being conducted in a laboratory and not in the actual museum. This would seem to make the experience inauthentic, as it was not in the actual environment that it was supposed to study. Although, the experience could be labeled authentic, in that families (learners) were engaged in real activity.

The article by Zimmerman, Reeve and Bell (2009) once again finds our setting in a science museum, this time looking at epistemic resources that individuals use while engaging in learning activities. This article focused on biology (for once we are not reading about physics!) and how families make use of knowledge in forming new knowledge based on museum exhibits. Coincidentally, we see a focus on the individual, social, and cultural influences during their museum interaction which once again hints at Vygotsky’s theory. Key to their belief is what is called Everyday Expertise (p.480) which is used to analyze the individual, social, and cultural influences on various learning processes. Experienced museum-going families were the research subjects in this article. Data was provided by analyzing video recordings of families going through the museum, pre-visit interviews, and post-visit interviews.  Situated learning was mentioned and many examples of transfer were documented. This would appear to support the middle of the road solution to the argument between the cognitive and situated camps from our previous readings in class.

Roth and Van Eijck (2010) will garner the least of my attention for this week’s response. Maybe I was at my limits of reading capacity. A proposal for a relevant unit for planning for, researching, and analyzing STEM activities was suggested by the authors. I will say that I was interested in the following: knowledgeability, debrouillard/e and knowledge as collective property that focus on knowledge not being static. Once again, we see Vygotsky’s influence in their work. It would appear that the authors are also promoting situated learning and often refer to “real-life” applications and learning as opposed to schooling.

Overall a good set of readings.

Is it me, or is Lev Vygotsky omnipresent?

 


17
Oct 10

SciEd — informal science learning

It was interesting reading the Zimmerman, Reeve and Bell, and Gutwill and Allen articles together and comparing their approaches to their studies of families’ experiences with museum exhibits.  Both seem to have the goal of improving museum learning, and both seem to be viewing learning from more than one approach, though they leaned towards the socio-cultural or situated views of learning.  Zimmerman et al. explicitly state that they use the cognitive, situated and socio-cultural approaches, and call this the Everyday Expertise framework.  They certainly use a mix of terms from the different approaches, for example, they relate a child explaining a concept to his mother as building identity within a community of practice, and mention guided participation.  But I struggled to see their connections to the cognitive approach, other than they mention transfer of knowledge, and seem to imply that museum information can be distributed to museum-goers. Gutwill and Allen are not as explicit in the approach they use, but focus specifically on inquiry, which I believe is connected to constructivism and builds on the construction of knowledge through collaboration, interaction and participation by the learner to build their knowledge, rather than just absorbing information.  They also state that inquiry games rely on “cognitive scaffolding” (p. 715) from the adults, but also mention that individuals monitor their knowledge, their conceptual understanding, and the changes within it, which sounds more like the cognitive approach. 

Overall, I thought the Gutwill and Allen study was a much more useful study because they acknowledged more realistic and practical aspects of learning in a museum, such as congestion, design of the exhibits, how families may differ in their activities in the museum, and effects of demographics.  And, Gutwill and Allen’s study seems to be more useful to a museum for designing exhibits that can promote inquiry learning and for shedding light on the role of museum guides in the process.  Zimmerman et al. on the other hand, were more descriptive, and it seemed almost as if they expected learning that happens in a museum environment to somehow be different from learning that happens in any other setting.  But it was interesting that they both seem to draw the conclusion that parents need help with encouraging learning for their children.  Gutwill and Allen acknowledge the role of the parents as “natural guides” (p. 738), but who may be lacking content knowledge and skills for promoting inquiry.  And Zimmerman et al. acknowledge the variation in parents’ abilities, so that parents may need something like cheat-sheets for some content.

The Roth and Van Eijck article was interesting to compare to Zimmerman et al. as far as how each deals with traditional learning theories.  Zimmerman et al. seem to accept parts of several of the traditional learning theories and group them all together into one unified framework, whereas Roth and Van Eijck reject traditional learning perspectives, especially constructivism and cognitive approaches, because they do not explain the dilemma-driven, learn-as-you-go approach, and they name their new approach the “Total Life” concept.  But still, they use terminology and ideas from socio-cultural learning and Vygotsky, cultural-historical activity theory, and situated learning, and they specifically make references to the lack of transfer from classroom to environment in which the information is needed, learning-while-doing, whole person acting in the world, collaborative learning, and the importance of context-dependent information.  Both Roth and Van Eijck and Zimmerman et al. seem to change ingredients or the amount of ingredients in the recipe just enough to warrant a new name for it.  But, anyway, I agreed with Roth and Van Eijck that their approach is definitely a way that individuals can learn, and I think the most compelling part was how much motivation is valued in it.  But I questioned whether this could apply to learning for all ages, it seems more applicable to older learners (not young children), though they state that is applies to all ages. 


17
Oct 10

Family Learning Time

  I had a lot of trouble getting much out of the Roth article, mainly just because I had a hard time getting interested in it, so I will focus on the Gutwill and Zimmerman articles. 

  I found the Gutwill article actually very interesting.  They had an interesting way of going about investigating the use of inquiry.  I can understand why it is important to investigate something of the nature.  I have found myself sometimes going through museums and just kind of passing through and not really putting much thought into it.  I don’t know that I can say for sure if some of that was not due to disinterest in the specific topic, but having the families participate in the way they did was a good way to make things interactive.  It makes sense that after encouraging the PA and IR skills that the families would have increased numbers in those areas as opposed to the families that were not exposed to the ideas.  However, I find one thing about the whole experiment that bothers me is the fact that at the posttest, the families were asked to use the method that they were taught specifically.  There are some issues with this as far as I am concerned.  If they were asked to use it, then it is difficult to know whether they used it because they actually found it helpful and worthwhile or whether they just used it because they were asked to and they didn’t want to disappoint the educator.  I realize that the authors discuss this in the article and also discuss why they chose to do this but I feel that it has sort of a comprimising effect on the actual data and conclusions.

   The Zimmerman article relates nicely to the Gutwill article.  Both are based on the interactions of families in museums and their learning as a result.  The Zimmerman article approaches the idea kind of in a backwards fashion from the Gutwill article.  The Gutwill article first teaches/introduces strategies such as the formulation of questions and interpretation of results; whereas, Zimmerman et al. approach it from a view of ideas and experiences that the families bring with them and connect to the exhibits in the museum.  I think both are interesting to investigate.  I think the approach Zimmerman et al. takes shows a demonstration of how people’s views and experiences may determine how they see things in the world.  The articles clearly states that they have a situated viewpoint and also think that experiences depend on “cultural tools such as languages, technologies, dispositions…” (pg 481).  It is a good example of using connections and different signs and tools to help understand other ideas that they may be less familiar with.


17
Oct 10

SCIED – Science Learning in Everyday Life

                Each of the three readings for this week provided three distinct perspectives on how people learn. I enjoyed reading some articles that were out of my normal scope of curriculum and instruction.  Attending to the theoretical frameworks used in the articles helped me connect what we have been talking and reading about for class to actual educational research.  It was helpful to get more examples of how authors actually used different theoretical frameworks to shape their studies.    I will discuss the articles and their corresponding theoretical frameworks in order of how we learned the theories in class: from cognitive to situated. 

             Though the Gutwell and Allen article states “learning in museums is a fundamentally social experience, often taking place in multigenerational groups that draw on each member’s interests and expertise as the group interacts with exhibits” (Gutwell and Allen 2009, pg. 711),  the authors take a cognitive view of learning. They do stress, however, the importance of the individual in a social setting. The authors talk of “prediction and metacognition” as well as grounding their theories in “cognitive science” and “sociocultural theory.”  Some of the authors’ goals include supporting metacognition as well as minimizing cognitive load on museum visitors learning inquiry skills.   Also, the authors recognize that each individual may have specific learning agendas and focus on these as a goal rather than increasing participation in a community.  Ultimately, the authors’ are trying to implement cognitive skill sets to aid inquiry through game play.  Though I’m not exactly sure on the separation between quantitative and qualitative data analysis (for example, is it qualitative analysis when you quantify qualitative data, such as ranking interview responses?), this article seemed to focus heavily on statistical analysis and contained pages of data tables, which we learned in class are more indicative of a cognitive theoretical framework than a situated framework.

                The second article by Zimmerman et al. seemed to pick and choose from both the cognitive and situated camps.  The authors use an “Everyday Expertise” framework to ground their study. They state, “We argue that individual and cognitive aspects of learning are fundamentally connected to the social and cultural aspects of learning; therefore, we analyze the intertwining role of individual cognitive resources, situated activities, and cultural toolkit resources that support learning interactions and processes.” (Zimmerman et al.  2009, pg. 478) They claim belief in both cognitive and situated learning within the same statement.   Throughout the article, the authors pull from both views almost equally.  They write “Finally, there are studies in many fields of individual and social aspects of learning taken separately; our work, in contrast, uses ethnographic methods and an Everyday Expertise analytical framework to connect individual and social perspectives.”  (Zimmerman et al. 2009, pg. 479).  This viewpoint confused me, because, I thought you weren’t allowed to agree with both cognitive and situated theoretical frameworks at the same time.  We are sort of expected to choose (and argue) one or the other, but Zimmerman et al. seem to use both simultaneously.  I thought that individual cognitive and situated cognitive theories can’t just be swapped out for the other depending on the situation, or am I just confused?  

                The last article by Roth and Van Eijck was extremely interesting.  I have never heard of the “Whole Life” theoretical framework before.  Roth writes “In the totality of life, thought reproduces itself just-in-time and then disappears again when the relevant and salient episode recedes into the past to be recounted and accounted for in stories…” (Roth and Van Eijck 2010 pg.  1018)  Though Roth and Eijck reject previous theoretical frameworks, their argument is definitely based on the situated cognition end of the individual/situated spectrum.  Instead of learning occurring in communities of practice, however, learning cannot be reduced to just the community; the entire life of a person needs to be considered when viewing how people learn, not just their relation to a particular community.  Though Roth and Van Eijck do not present any data ( this article is a commentary, not a study), they do present lengthy anecdotes.   The authors present a Utopian view of “whole life” learning which may break down when pushed to further study.  I am wondering how you could really go about studying someone’s learning through the perspective of his entire life.  You could make a lot of claims to fit your argument, but how difficult would it be to collect the data?


17
Oct 10

Learning outside the classroom

          This week’s articles from Science Education were interesting to read because they did not focus on the typical type of learning that we generally think about as classroom learning.  I was especially interested in the two on learning in museums/science centers.  We know that they are considered ‘educational field trips’.  As a student, I remember a trip to the Carnegie Science Center in Pittsburgh but I don’t remember the education emphasis of the trip.  When I hear the word museum, the image that I immediately think of is a large building with multiple large rooms where people go to look at paintings and sculptures in almost complete silence.  This is certainly not the case for science centers.

          The article that I learned the most from was the Gutwill & Allen article entitled “Facilitating Family Group Inquiry at Science Museum Exhibits”.  I found the research behind this article to be intriguing.  I thought the choice of ‘inquiry games’ that they had families engage in were good choices.  I was pleased to see that they had a control group and a group that had a tour leader.  I think that one additional group that may be interesting to see is how well the visitors would ‘score’ if the leader was engaging the families as he/she normally would.  I think it would be a way to gauge how well the guides at the Exploratorium are engaging the visitors and encouraging them to learn during their visit.

          The two inquiry games that they encouraged the families participate in, to me, were extremely different.  The Juicy Question game encouraged everyone in the family, not just the adults or the kids, to share what they were thinking and to build off of each other’s ideas.  This was a technique that could be used in a classroom setting as well to guide students in a lab.  I thought this was a more ‘authentic’ demonstration of how science works.  This also encouraged them to critically think about what they were doing and not just seen and exhibit and think ‘this is neat’ and move on.  To me, this seemed to be the more reasonable of the two games that were played because it encouraged everyone to participate and talk about what they saw or reasons behind it.  However, this technique (and the other) still received criticism from the participants that “sometimes it seemed a little forced” (Gutwill & Allen, 2009, p.740).  This would certainly be a valid complaint if your intention when coming to the Exploratorium was to have fun and maybe learn a little bit.

          The other inquiry game, Hands-Off, felt more structured to me.  By having to say ‘hands-off’ in order to try something seemed much more structured and demanding then Juicy Question.  Perhaps it is the slightly negative connotation that I associate with ‘hands-off’ that made me not support this style as much.  I felt that it was not as encouraging for everyone to participate because when it came time to try something, it was a one person show.  To me, this was more of a focus on taking turns than a focus on the activities or learning.  This was also reflected in Table 3 with their responses during the posttest interview (Gutwill & Allen, 2009, p. 731).  When looking at this table, it is clear that the emphasis of each of the experimental groups was different:  Juicy Question focused on the activity, Hands-Off on taking turns, and the exhibit tour on the history.  Therefore, depending on what you wanted to learn from the experience, different types of groups may work better than others.  Similar to how a student’s learning style has to somehow match what the teaching style is in order for the child to learn the most they can.

          The other article that I want to briefly mention is the Zimmerman et al article.  This article followed families through the entire museum to see how they interested with the exhibits and each other.  After reading the Gutwill and Allen piece where they looked at four exhibits, this was surprising to me.  For me, by letting them choose where they want to go and what exhibits intrigues them, this type of research could give a better picture of how families are actually interacting with exhibits.  I felt that this would be a more accurate view than only looking at 4 specific exhibits.  An analogy that I was relating this to was you would not judge someone’s knowledge in chemistry to give them four questions.


17
Oct 10

JRST Articles

This was a very interesting experience this week.  Reading the three JRST articles, I realized that I have never really considered researchers’ theoretical frameworks very deeply.  I have read a lot of educational research articles over the past several years, but I think I have always taken the theoretical underpinnings as matter of fact.  I certainly have never considered them as I did this week, having spent the last seven weeks reading about, writing about, and discussing theories of learning and knowing.  I now understand that theoretical frameworks can be very different and they provide the lens through which your entire study is filtered.  If I am being honest, I used to think that the explanation of theoretical framework found in a paper was just there to reference previous works and to fill up some space.  The importance is now much clearer to me.  It underpins one’s entire study because when you take a stance on your views of learning and knowing, it illuminates what you find interesting, what you choose to study, how you choose to study it, what you think is important, and how you think education can be improved.

All three of the articles from JRST were dealing with practice.  As I mentioned above, my previous consideration of theoretical frameworks was minimal.  I thought it was really cool how easy it was to understand them now!  From the title and abstract alone, one can generally infer the authors’ point of view.  The following are portions of the titles:  “Constructing an idea; shared intellectual property; drama activities; argumentation; questions and discursive interaction.”  From this, I could see that the articles all have a strong social and situated view of learning.  I may have subconsciously understood that before this course, but now it is so easy to see.  Their theories of learning almost jump off the page!

In discussing the social nature of learning and their research goals, Verelas et al. even state that: “We did not address individual children’s understandings and participation in the drama activities, but rather how ideas, actions, behaviors, and emotions were collectively enacted, developed, and transformed” (p. 306).  I think that the authors still see value in cognitive processes for explaining some aspects of learning, but they also recognize the critical importance of the social aspect.  Evidence for this is that they often reference the abundance of research that has already been conducted on the cognitive side: “Science learning standards and educational research have developed good tools for understanding the individual, cognitive side of the process, but relatively few for studying science learning in classrooms as a shared, social process of knowledge construction.” (Radinsky et al., p. 621).  Finally, even though the article by Chin and Osborne felt much more quantitative in some of its methods (especially compared to the other two clearly qualitative articles), it still had a social learning framework as indicated by their stated purpose: “To create the conditions for collaborative discourse that focuses on questioning and argumentation, by having students pose questions and respond to each other when they were put together in small groups to discuss opposing viewpoints” (p. 884).


Skip to toolbar