Another Tale by H.Smith
This week’s reading’s (Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989; NRC, 2007) showed a distinct progression in the understanding of how students learn and the nature of knowledge itself. From the learning models of Skinner (1954) and Posner et. al (1981), I can appreciate how the idea of situated cognition has come about. Among many reasons, this aims to address the disparity in how school culture typically divulges facts and ‘knowledge’ to students, resulting in at best, the learning of discrete problem-solving skills that rarely translate into real-world skills and application.
The recognition of knowledge as being culture based and learned within a community of practice stands out to me as being defining differences in what literature has discussed up until this point. The idea that school itself is a culture of practice with its own implicit rules and regulations makes complete sense; no other time in our lives, unless wait…we are becoming teachers, ok nevermind…, will we be required to engage with knowledge as a student does during formative schooling. Behavioral differences in how an individual engages with knowledge are examined by (Brown, Collins & Duguid 1989) when comparing just plain folks (JPFs), practitioners, and students. Unlike JPFs, students reason with laws, act on symbols and attempt to answer well-defined problems. For the student in school, the goal is to produce the correct answer that has a fixed meaning before they can progress onto the next problem or class. Although a student may master linear algebra or fractions in class, upon entering the real world, when faced with emergent problems in a context that is different from schooling, students tend to struggle with applying their schooling knowledge to find solutions, ‘school activity too often tends to be hybrid, implicitly framed by one culture, but explicitly attributed to another,’ (Brown, Collins and Duguid 1989, p 33). This is also supported by the NRC’s TSS chapter considering the nature of knowledge in the science classroom, ‘science taught in schools is often different from actual science and from everyday life. Students’ learning difficulties are thus increased because scientific goals are distorted and scientific ways of thinking are inadequately taught,’ (NRC 2007, p.178). I can recall from my own schooling, the question often being asked ‘when will we ever need this in the real world?’ Schooling at present fails to recreate the community of practice that exists in the relevant subject domain outside the classroom. Brown, Collins, and Duguid (1989) discuss this via the tool analogy that explains how one can learn all there is to know about using a tool, but if the individual is not engaging within that community of practice in a tactile, real-world sense, it is unlikely they can apply their knowledge and put the tool to good use.
The issue now is to create a school culture that allows for some form of enculturation for students within the discipline of the subject. Designing class to emulate how mathematicians, engineers, and scientists engage with problem-solving is outlined within the cognitive apprenticeship model. I need to say that I consider the heart of the cognitive apprenticeship model to be a far more appropriate method of teaching. However, I do wonder how well we can use this idea of knowledge enculturation within classrooms when we as teachers have likely not participated in this model of learning before. Steering clear of facts and memorization has immeasurable merit for when a student enters the real world, but that’s the point, for schools, this is often immeasurable. Unfortunately, when we still demand high scores on standardized tests, will we be able to move away from teaching ‘to the test’ so to speak? Teachers would themselves require a re-schooling on how certain communities of practice engage with knowledge because I know myself that I do not know, on a deep level, how a mathematician engages with real-world problems, and it has not been until I undertook research as part of my undergraduate that I felt I had engaged with a community of science practice.
A question I have now, is that in going with this model of cognitive apprenticeship, is it all or nothing? I ask this because nearly all actual apprenticeships do have some form of schooling, I look at how tradespeople in Australia undergo training with the initial period of time being in a classroom at trade-school, learning fundamentals before they are allowed to be on site and under supervision. Creating schooling environments that can allow for scaffolded forms of cognitive apprenticeship is perhaps a way to reduce the reliance on textbook teaching?
References:
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning. Educational Researcher, 18(1), 32–42.
National Research Council. (2007). Participation in scientific practices and discourse. Taking Science to School: Learning and Teaching Science in Grades K-8, 186–210.
National Research Council. (2007). Understanding how scientific knowledge is constructed. Taking Science to School: Learning and Teaching Science in Grades K-8, 168–185.