Rather than process the assigned articles in the order they were given, I decided to read them chronologically. By doing this, I was able to observe the changes taking place in teaching philosophy throughout the decades, as well as the changing focus of science education.
It was surprising to me that Dewey’s article, even though almost a century-old, proved that intellectual minds were already debating the most practical methods of education. It was interesting to see him debate the existence of a science of education being that science suggests the existence of systematic methods of inquiry (Dewey, 1929, pg. 8). It was quite amazing to see Dewey philosophically define science as a process that “makes for diversification rather than uniformity” (Dewey, 1929, pg. 12), but then explain its potential pitfalls to those teachers who seek out recipes on how to do things with guaranteed success (Dewey, 1929, pg. 14). I admittedly find myself typically working to embrace the science of teaching, but after reading Dewey’s chapter, I think it is best to accept that teaching requires the artistic creativity that Dewey, Skinner, and the NRC suggest.
Skinner’s reading concerned me in the beginning, as his methods of training humans were reminiscent of Pavlov’s experimentation on dogs. When he was suggesting that reinforcement could shape up an organism’s behavior at will (Skinner, 1954, pg. 87), I began to assume that he was going to tout that educators should aim to have their students become good at repetition. He does suggest that there is a place and time for this sort of reinforcement in education, specifically mathematics, and that the current consensus was that students of the time needed this kind of structuring to become successful at math. I find it interesting that this type of education parallels what I commonly know as “plug and chug” methods that students learn in STEM courses that require students to commit formulas to memory and then apply them to completely numerical problems. It makes me wonder if Skinner may have been a proponent of creating the all too familiar plug and chug paradigm we see teachers working to abolish today. Later in the article, Skinner did put me at ease by admitting that his reinforcement device would act as a replacement teacher, but as a tool to allow educators to focus more time on interactions with students that would help build their relationship (Skinner, 1954, pg. 96). To put it concisely, it is absolutely crazy to me to think about the amount of work Skinner put into proving the idea of utilizing what is rudimentary learning software (but in his time was hardware), to give a teacher more time to interact with students.
I found that reading the literature by Dewey and Skinner helped me appreciate the depth put forth by the NRC in the framework. Although the educational concepts put forth by Dewey and Skinner are not nearly as robust as the NRC’s it is important to note that without the scaffolding they put forth, education could still be as rudimentary as having students memorizing math facts. Dewey and the NRC seemed to have more similarities between their educational ideologies though, as Dewey and the NRC suggest that educators should be flexible with their methods of teaching. The NRC does slightly oppose Dewey’s notion that implementing science-based rules of education without augmentation is risky. The NRC outlines phenomena that are appropriate to be covered from grades k-2, 3-5, 6-8, and 9-12 (NRC, 2012, pgs. 33-34) which makes me question what Dewey would think.
Overall, I thought that the 3 readings worked really well together and provided an abridged timeline of proposed methodologies of education. It was enlightening to read some early work being done by innovative educational researchers as well as the subsequent foundation laid by the NRC decades later. Who would have thought that educators have been combatting plug and chug since the 1920s.