The well researched topic of fundamental attribution error, which is the tendency to overemphasize personal characteristics (internal attributions) and disregard situational factors (external attributions) when judging peoples’ behavior, often leads people to instantly mischaracterize others for being inherently bad (Gawronski, 2007). Despite this well- established cognitive process, people still believe they consider external factors more than others (Van Boven et al., 2003), making awareness and combating this process more critical. When simply listening to lectures or reading articles, students attributed the information given to them as representative of the educator/writer’s opinions, resulting in corresponding assumptions, whether positive or negative (Coren, 1993; Jones & Harris, 1967). Gilbert et al. (1988) found that the fundamental attribution error is an automatic cognitive process, in which people must make significant efforts to avoid. Therefore, many groups use the concept of giving the benefit of the doubt, which helps consider what may have caused someone to partake in a certain scenario or express a given opinion.
Although the English idiom is said to be traced back to the 1700’s (O’Neill, 1770), benefit of the doubt, as a concept, has been both a religious and cultural practice for substantially longer, as it is referenced in Christian, Jewish, and Muslim sacred texts (Illuminating Lamp, 2009; Wollenberg, 2010). Teamwork and healthy relationships heavily rely on trusting one another’s intentions and taking differences into account, without attributing internal underlying negative factors to them. The vulnerability loop, which describes taking the risk in trusting someone, often encourages similar assurances (Neale et al., 2003), essentially creating a prosocial trusting environment between those involved. By simply accepting that people have good intentions, are trying their best, and/or have an explanation, one can add so much to the integrity of the relationship, instead of taking from it, subsequently decreasing negative attitudes and animosity. This may also constitute as being charitable, a necessary deed in many religions, where the kindest explanations are sought instead of forcing people to prove themselves.
Judaism and Islam are largely associated with collectivism, as they both thrive for dedicated community and prioritize proper relationship functioning over their own idiosyncratic goals (Cohen et al., 2016). That said, as it is the culture, in collectivist regions, people tend to steer away from blaming individuals on their personal failures or give personal credit for success, rather, they favor external over internal attributions (Lee et al., 1996). This difference in error of attribution depends on the cultural context, but can likely be influenced by religion, as well, by following the concept of giving the benefit of the doubt. Hence, by combating the fundamental attribution error and modeling communities that don’t allow for it, people are encouraged to decrease bias, suggestive beliefs, and focus on values. This method highlights a unique way of thinking which allows context to be considered, a practice highly desired in clinical psychology training (Hunt et al., 2019).
Combating this type of error in attribution even extends to marriage satisfaction and the challenges that may arise within personal relationships. While there is more concern for psychological intimacy and emotional excitement in marriage among individualistic culture, collectivist culture emphasized the importance of life-long commitment and cultural traditions (Lutzow, 2011), likely influencing the success rate of their marriages. Despite having some of the suspected highest infidelity numbers in the world (Crompton, 2015), collectivist countries positively predicted marriage quality (Cirhinlioğlu et al., 2019). This is possibly caused by attributing such actions to external factors and context, instead of seeing the perpetrator as inherently bad or having unfavorable motive. Moreover, people from individualistic cultures may tend to favor internal attributions for the wives’ behavior in this case and see her as weak or lacking self-respect. Just like the vulnerability loop, which enables trust and confidence, the fundamental attribution error can lead to a devasting loop of pointing out victims and misplacing blame. Religions and collectivist cultures alike show the importance of not falling victim to this error, and it’s crucial that people make efforts to overcome this biased mode of viewing others. The benefit of the doubt isn’t just a religious theme, but a moral one, which everyone should be educated on and be encouraged to practice, whether it’s inherently part of the culture or not.
References
Cirhinlioğlu, F. G., Özdikmenli-Demir, G., Kindap Tepe, Y., & Cirhinlioğlu, Z. C. (2019). Marital Quality, Individualism/Collectivism and Divorce Attitude in Turkey. International Journal of Innovative Science and Research Technology , 4(3), 559–566. https://ijisrt.com/wp-content/uploads/2019/04/IJISRT19MA138.pdf
Cohen, A. B., Wu, M. S., & Miller, J. (2016). Religion and Culture: Individualism and Collectivism in the East and West. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 47(9), 1236–1249. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022116667895
Coren, S. (1993). When teaching is evaluated on political grounds. Academic Questions, 6.
Crompton, P. (2015, August 20). Saudis, Moroccans are the biggest “cheaters,” Ashley Madison hack reveals. Al Arabiya English. https://english.alarabiya.net/variety/2015/08/20/Saudis-Moroccans-are-the-biggest-cheaters-Ashley-Madison-hack-reveals-
Gawronski, B. (2007). Fundamental attribution error. Sage.
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 733–740. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.733
Hunt, M. G., Miller, R. A., Stacy, M. A., Lynam, S., & Carr, E. R. (2019). Public servant, silent servant: A call to action for advocacy training in public service settings. Psychological Services, 17(1). https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000387
Illuminating Lamp. (2009). Benefit of doubt. Siraajunmuneer.wordpress.com. https://siraajunmuneer.wordpress.com/tag/benefit-of-doubt/
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0
Lee, F., Hallahan, M., & Herzog, T. (1996). Explaining Real-Life Events: How Culture and Domain Shape Attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(7), 732–741. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167296227007
Lutzow, D. (2011). Online dating – A cross-cultural comparison of matchmaking websites in the United States of America, Germany, India, and Japan [Thesis]. https://www.grin.com/document/207098
Neale, M., Polzer, J., & Mannix, E. (Eds.). (2003). Identity Issues in Groups. In Research on Managing Groups and Teams. JAI Press.
O’Neill, S. C. (1770). The summary of the Boston massacre trial. Bostonmassacre.net; Boston Massacre Historical Society. http://www.bostonmassacre.net/trial/index.htm
Van Boven, L., White, K., Kamada, A., & Gilovich, T. (2003). Intuitions about situational correction in self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 249–258. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.249
Wollenberg, M. (2010). Benefit of the Doubt. Chabad.org. https://www.chabad.org/parshah/article_cdo/aid/403659/jewish/Benefit-of-the-Doubt.htm